The peaks of active traveling waves also shifted basally as the s

The peaks of active traveling waves also shifted basally as the stimulus level increased (Figures S1D and S1E). The high gain and nonlinearity were completely abolished when the active process was interrupted by anoxia (Figure 1E), which additionally displaced the wave’s peak toward the cochlear base. The phase profiles of traveling waves displayed slopes that were dependent on stimulus level in healthy cochleas

but not following anoxia (Figure S1F). These phenomena reflect the loss after anoxia of a tuned, Selleck Talazoparib tonotopically distributed amplification mechanism that enhances a traveling wave as it approaches the characteristic place at which it peaks. To investigate the interplay between active cellular forces and the spatial shaping of an active traveling wave, we developed an optical technique that locally and significantly perturbs electromotility. Small carboxylic acids inhibit prestin-based motility; salicylate Rucaparib in vitro is the most effective of these blockers (Tunstall et al., 1995; Oliver et al., 2001). Our technique uses 4-azidosalicylate, the azide group of which forms covalent bonds upon activation by ultraviolet (UV) light

(Figures 2A and 2B). The compound is therefore an inhibitor that forms an irreversible complex with prestin, effectively disabling it. We initially characterized the effect of 4-azidosalicylate on somatic motility in HEK293T cells transfected with prestin-eGFP. Motility was deduced from measurements of a cell’s voltage-dependent capacitance, which reflects the gating currents that accompany conformational changes in large ensembles unless of prestin molecules. Capacitance was measured from phase changes in the currents elicited by sinusoidal membrane-potential perturbations at different holding potentials (Fidler and Fernandez, 1989). When

washed onto prestin-transfected HEK293T cells, 4-azidosalicylate largely abolished somatic motility as inferred from the linearization of the voltage-capacitance relation, an effect that was reversible upon washout (Figure 2C). UV irradiation had no effect on motility in control medium (Figure 2D). If a cell incubated in 4-azidosalicylate was exposed to UV light, however, motility did not return after washout (Figures 2E and 2F). The cell nonetheless remained healthy as assessed by visual appearance and by the absence of leakage currents. Because the nonlinear capacitance measured in prestin-expressing cells cannot be dissociated from mechanical motility (Santos-Sacchi, 1991), photoinactivation presumably elicits a concurrent attenuation of the latter. We nonetheless confirmed that photoinactivation affects the somatic motility of isolated outer hair cells.

, 2008) We also expect further developments in calcium imaging t

, 2008). We also expect further developments in calcium imaging technology, especially concerning devices capable of 3D imaging and miniaturized devices to be used in freely moving animals. Finally, calcium imaging may greatly benefit from the development of improved GECIs with Selleckchem NVP-AUY922 higher signal sensitivity and better temporal response characteristics. We thank Jia Lou for excellent

technical assistance. This work was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (IRTG 1373), the ERA-Net Program, the CIPSM cluster, and the Schiedel Foundation. A.K. is a Carl-von-Linde Senior Fellow of the Institute for Advanced Study of the TUM. “
“Despite intensive study over the past three decades, neurodegenerative diseases remain insufficiently understood, precluding rational design of therapeutic interventions that can reverse or even arrest the progressive loss of neurological function. The identification and study of genetic mutations responsible for numerous neurodegenerative syndromes have led to several compelling theories on disease pathogenesis. Some of these theories, such as those involving a central see more role for protein misfolding, mitochondrial dysfunction, oxidative stress, excitotoxicity, and transcriptional dysregulation, have been proposed for a wide variety of neurodegenerative disorders. Data supporting a role for each of these pathogenic processes in a variety

of clinical syndromes has been generated from primary patient material (usually postmortem), in vitro cell culture, and primary neuron models, invertebrate and vertebrate model systems, and most recently, induced pluripotent stem cell modeling. These wide-ranging disease studies, coupled with powerful model systems, have yielded a wealth of information regarding pathways of neuronal demise in the face of mutant gene expression and have revealed neuroprotective mechanisms that effectively

counter pathogenic cellular processes. However, even with this wealth of information, effective interventions for these diseases remain agonizingly elusive. Why? One potential answer to this question is that experimentation into the basis of neuronal degeneration and death has generated hypotheses of pathogenesis that Sitaxentan are overly focused on (1) late-stage events and (2) events that are best modeled in isolated neurons. For example, many groups have promoted the idea that protein misfolding is a critical step in neurodegeneration. This theory posits that once the capacity of a neuron to handle misfolded proteins is exceeded, mitochondrial dysfunction results, thereby promoting increased oxidative stress—which in turn promotes the further accumulation of damaged proteins that must be handled by an already overwhelmed protein degradation system (Saxena and Caroni, 2011 and Williams and Paulson, 2008).

While the acute stress response is an important and necessary mec

While the acute stress response is an important and necessary mechanism to adapt

to environmental changes that occur throughout life thus promoting effective coping, severe or chronic stress can result in allostatic load and is also a contributing risk factor for the development of several psychiatric disorders such as depression and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (McEwen and Wingfield, 2003 and McEwen, 2007). However, it is also important to note that many stress-exposed individuals do not develop stress-related psychiatric Navitoclax ic50 disorders (Charney and Manji, 2004, Yehuda and LeDoux, 2007 and Caspi et al., 2003) and are thus more resilient to the negative consequences of stress than others.

Resilience to stress is the ability to cope with environmental challenges, ensuring survival, while susceptibility to the negative consequences of stress seems to result from an improper functioning of the systems of resilience or an amplification of the stress experience (Karatsoreos and McEwen, 2013), which in turn can result in maladaptive physiological and behavioural responses. Such maladaptive responses to stress may increase the risk for the development of stress-related psychiatric disorders, and as such great effort is being made to elucidate the neural processes that underlie stress-resilience in the hope GSK1120212 that these might be then exploited for drug development (Franklin Tamara et al., 2012, Russo et al., 2012, Wu et al., 2013 and Hughes, 2012). The hippocampus is a key brain area involved in the regulation of the stress response, exerting negative feedback on the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis (Jacobson and Sapolsky, 1991), the system within the body responsible for the release of glucocorticoid stress hormones. Stressors rapidly stimulate the secretion of corticotropin-releasing

factor and vasopressin from parvocellular neurons of the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus and this stimulates the release of adrenocorticotropic hormone from the anterior pituitary, which in turn stimulates the release of MTMR9 glucocorticoid stress hormones from the adrenal cortex into the circulation (Cullinan et al., 1995). These glucocorticoids, cortisol in humans and corticosterone in rodents (Herman and Cullinan, 1997), feedback onto two types of receptors in the brain: the mineralocorticoid receptors – MR and glucocorticoid receptors – GR, which are highly expressed in limbic structures of the brain, including the hippocampus (Morimoto et al., 1996). While hippocampal MR mediates the effects of glucocorticoids on assessment of the stressor and initiation of the stress response, GR acts in the consolidation of acquired information (de Kloet et al., 2005 and De Kloet et al., 1998).

In order to quantify the amount of information carried by differe

In order to quantify the amount of information carried by different 3 MA response variables (i.e., latency, peak timing and spike counts), we performed a decoding analysis to ask how accurately an ideal observer could classify each individual trial as belonging to one of six odor stimuli. By comparing decoding accuracy using vectors consisting of different variables derived from aPC responses, we compared the relative importance of each coding strategy. As decoders (ideal observers), we used

linear classifiers including perceptrons and support vector machines with linear kernels. These decoders essentially calculate a weighted sum of inputs followed by a threshold and therefore resemble a biophysical decoding of aPC information that might actually be implemented in downstream areas. Input codes based on the total number or rate of spikes in a sniff cycle provided the most reliable performance in odor classification, whereas codes based on first spike latency or peak timing performed significantly worse (Figure 4E). Furthermore,

combining latency or peak timing with rate failed to improve decoding accuracy. Although it has been postulated that spike times may provide a more rapid coding mechanism (Cury and Uchida, 2010; Gollisch and Meister, 2008; Thorpe et al., 2001), we found that decoders using spike count actually performed faster than those based on spike latency or peak timing (Figure 4F), demonstrating that spike counts can convey information both more quickly and in a more reliable manner. Furthermore, PFI-2 supplier decoding based on complete temporal patterns of activity in a sniff cycle did little to improve decoding accuracy (Figure 4G). Finally,

using phase of spike occurrence with respect to sniffing cycle instead of absolute time did not improve the decoding accuracy (Figure 4H). Together, these results suggest that spike rates Carnitine palmitoyltransferase II or counts are the predominant carrier of olfactory information in the aPC, and that the dependence of odor coding on spike timing is greatly reduced compared to the olfactory bulb (Cury and Uchida, 2010). We next compared the performance of aPC populations decoded using linear classifiers to the performance of the animal. Decoding based on total spike counts in the first sniff using the entire 179 neurons gave nearly perfect performance on pure odors (Figures 5A and 5B). For both pure and mixture stimuli, the accuracy of the classifier reached a level comparable to that of the animal using only about 70 neurons (Figure 5A). Analysis of the time course of decoding using a short sliding time window showed that the maximum information could be read out from the initial burst of activity within 100 ms after the first inhalation onset and that the rate of information dropped thereafter (Figures 5B and 5C).

Figures S3, S4, and S5) At individual recording sites in area LI

Figures S3, S4, and S5). At individual recording sites in area LIP, LFP activity at both 45 Hz and 15 Hz exhibited strong spatial tuning (Figure S3). Across the population of recording sites in area LIP, average LFP power at 15 Hz developed after target onset and differed according to whether or not a reach was made with a saccade (Figure 8A, memory period, p < < 0.001, rank-sum test). Gamma-band,

45 Hz, LFP power was Protein Tyrosine Kinase inhibitor directionally selective but did not depend on whether a coordinated saccade was made with the reach (Figure 8B, memory period, p = 0.74, rank-sum test). Consequently, selectivity of area LIP gamma-band LFP power for saccades does not change with a reach movement. Selleck MK0683 In contrast, beta-band LFP power in area LIP is selective for both movement direction and type, consistent with a role in the control of coordinated movements. Beta-band but not gamma-band LFP power in PRR showed similar signatures of coordination (Figures 8C, 8D, and S4). In contrast, in V3d, not only was there no movement specificity in beta-band signals, the initial significant decrease in beta-band selectivity immediately following target onset was not present (Figure 8E, 8F, and S5). Therefore, movement specificity of beta-band LFP power

is a feature of activity within PPC circuits and is not a global feature of brain activity. Here, we use a spike-field approach to identify a neural mechanism of coordination and find that only area LIP neurons

that coherently fire with beta-band LFP activity predict movement RT before coordinated movement. Decreasing beta-band activity speeds movement initiation. On average, RTs are faster on trials when there is less beta-band activity and slower on trials when there is more beta-band activity. Beta-band activity encodes the properties of coordinated movement (i.e., it is selective not only Thiamine-diphosphate kinase for the direction of the movement but also for determining whether a coordinated reach is made with a saccade). These properties of beta-band activity are a feature of area LIP and PRR and are not present in visual cortical areas. Therefore, we propose that posterior parietal beta-band activity coordinates the timing of reaches with saccades through the formation of a shared movement representation. To uncover the shared movement representation that is responsible for coordinated timing, we correlate the activity of individual neurons to nearby LFP activity. Our results demonstrate how the correlation of spiking with LFP activity can help us to define distinct neuronal populations in terms of the circuits in which they are active. By dividing neurons into two populations (i.e.

Note that predictions about the relative amplitudes of high and l

Note that predictions about the relative amplitudes of high and low frequencies in superficial and deep layers pertain to all frequencies—there is nothing in predictive coding per se to suggest characteristic frequencies in the gamma and beta ranges. However, one might speculate that the MAPK inhibitor characteristic frequencies

of canonical microcircuits have evolved to model and—through active inference—create the sensorium (Berkes et al., 2011; Engbert et al., 2011; Friston, 2010). Indeed, there is empirical evidence to support this notion in the visual (Lakatos et al., 2008; Meirovithz et al., 2012; Bosman et al., 2012) and motor (Gwin and Ferris, 2012) domain. In summary, predictions are formed by a linear accumulation of prediction errors. Conversely, prediction errors are nonlinear functions of predictions. This means that the conversion of prediction errors into predictions (Bayesian filtering) necessarily Selleck Bosutinib entails

a loss of high frequencies. However, the nonlinearity in the mapping from predictions to prediction errors means that high frequencies can be created (consider the effect of squaring a sine wave, which would convert beta into gamma). In short, prediction errors should express higher frequencies than the predictions that accumulate them. This is another example of a potentially important functional asymmetry between feedforward and feedback message passing that emerges under predictive coding. It is particularly interesting given recent evidence that feedforward connections may use higher frequencies than feedback connections (Bosman et al., 2012). In conclusion, there is a remarkable correspondence between the anatomy and physiology of the canonical microcircuit and the formal constraints implied by generalized predictive coding. Having said this, there are many variations on the mapping between computational and neuronal architectures: even if predictive coding is an appropriate implementation

of Bayesian filtering, there are many variations on the arrangement shown in Figure 5. For example, feedback connections could arise directly from cells encoding conditional expectations in supragranular layers. Indeed, there is emerging evidence that feedback connections between proximate hierarchical levels originate from both deep and superficial layers (Markov et al., 2011). Note until that this putative splitting of extrinsic streams is only predicted in the light of empirical constraints on intrinsic connectivity. One of our motivations—for considering formal constraints on connectivity—was to produce dynamic causal models of canonical microcircuits. Dynamic causal modeling enables one to compare different connectivity models, using empirical electrophysiological responses (David et al., 2006; Moran et al., 2008, 2011). This form of modeling rests upon Bayesian model comparison and allows one to assess the evidence for one microcircuit relative to another.

Insular damage (INS patients) was unilateral, covering almost 40%

Insular damage (INS patients) was unilateral, covering almost 40% of the functional AI, which was bilateral. Striatal degeneration

(PRE patients) was bilateral, but limited to 15% of the functional DS. In both cases, it remains difficult Linsitinib cell line to state what proportion of the ROI remained truly functional. The fact that we did observe the expected deficits suggests the ROI were significantly impaired, even if not entirely. The presence of brain damage outside the functional ROI raises the question of specificity, which we addressed by including control pathological conditions. We verified that, in both groups of interest (INS and PRE), functional punishment-related regions were more affected than functional reward-related regions, namely the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and ventral striatum. These groups therefore allowed testing the existence of opponent structures at both cortical

level with glioma and subcortical level with HD. Each group of interest was compared both to healthy controls and to patients who presented similar lesions, except that punishment-related ROI were not preferentially Selleck Entinostat affected. Thus, the observed deficits can be attributed to punishment-related ROI, perhaps not specifically to the clusters activated in fMRI analyses, but at least to anatomically defined AI and DS. Computational analyses revealed distinct deficits in INS and PRE patients. The flattened Oxalosuccinic acid punishment-learning curve following AI damage in INS patients was specifically captured by a reduced reinforcement magnitude in the loss condition. This means that not only learning was

slowed down, but also the asymptotic plateau was lower. It can be distinguished from a change in learning rate, which would only affect how fast the plateau is reached. This computational result suggests that AI damage attenuated not only signaling of aversive outcomes, but also signaling of aversive cues. This is consistent with our previous neuroimaging findings that the AI was responsive to both aversive outcome display (during learning period) and aversive cue display (during choice period). It is also more generally consistent with a number of neuroimaging studies that implicated the anterior insula in signaling aversive events (Büchel et al., 1998; Seymour et al., 2004; Nitschke et al., 2006; Samanez-Larkin et al., 2008; Kim et al., 2006, 2011). In our original fMRI analysis, as in some other fMRI studies (Kim et al., 2006; Seymour et al., 2004; Pessiglione et al., 2006), the AI was found to encode more precisely punishment prediction errors (received minus expected punishments) at the time of outcome. This is also consistent with the present computational result, as reducing reinforcement magnitude in the loss condition is one way to diminish punishment prediction error signal.

Although the clathrin-coated pits accumulating in the DKO had a s

Although the clathrin-coated pits accumulating in the DKO had a size similar to that of synaptic vesicles, they were less densely packed than synaptic vesicles (Figure 6B versus Figure 6C). Furthermore, the overall loss of synaptic vesicles was not fully accounted for by the increase in clathrin-coated pits (Figure 6F), suggesting that synaptic vesicle membranes might be partially trapped within the axonal plasma membrane outside of pits. Such a possibility agrees with the increased steady-state plasma membrane abundance of the vGlut1-pHlourin reporter in DKO neurons (Figure 4A). As a result, immunofluorescence for intrinsic membrane proteins

of synaptic vesicles (synaptophysin, synaptobrevin, synaptotagmin, and SV2), which are expected to be enriched in these pits, was less punctate in DKO FRAX597 molecular weight nerve terminals than in control

nerve terminals where the puncta correspond to abundant and highly clustered synaptic vesicles (Figure 7A and data not shown). However, not surprisingly, given the loss of synaptic vesicles, the most striking change was observed for synapsin 1 and Rab3a, two peripheral proteins of synaptic vesicles (De Camilli et al., 1990 and Fischer von Mollard et al., selleck screening library 1990) that dissociate from the vesicle prior to, or in parallel with, exocytosis and then reassociate with newly reformed synaptic vesicles once the endocytic/recycling journey is completed (Chi et al., 2001, Giovedi et al., 2004 and Star et al., 2005). Immunoreactivities for these proteins lost their normally highly punctate enrichment within presynaptic terminals and became more diffusely spread out along the axonal length (Figure 7A). Biochemical analysis of the subcellular localization

of synaptic vesicle proteins (synaptotagmin 1 and synaptophysin) by a cell surface biotinylation-based strategy already confirmed an increase in their plasma membrane levels (Figure 7B). The ultrastructural changes of many DKO nerve terminals described above (Figure 6) revealed a near-complete switch from a “secretion-ready mode” to an “endocytic mode,” where the large clusters of synaptic vesicles, which represent the defining morphological feature of synapses, are replaced by a massive accumulation of clathrin-coated pits. We asked whether these dramatic changes are reversible upon silencing of electrical activity (Ferguson et al., 2007). Neurons were first allowed to differentiate over 14 days in culture so that they would exhibit a strong accumulation of endocytic intermediates, and then exposed to tetrodotoxin (TTX; 1μM, overnight) to silence neuronal network activity. After TTX treatment, immunofluorescence for α-adaptin (and other clathrin coat components) became diffuse (Figures 8A–8C) and electron microscopy showed a reduction of clathrin-coated pit number (Figures 8D–8F).

Multiple classes of transmembrane subunits interacting within a n

Multiple classes of transmembrane subunits interacting within a native glutamate receptor complex appears to be an evolutionarily-conserved regulatory mechanism. Glutamate receptors in C. elegans are controlled by interactions among two classes of auxiliary subunits: suppressor of Lurcher (SOL)-1 and TARPs ( Wang et al., 2008). SOL-1 is a transmembrane CUB domain protein, unrelated to CNIH ( Zheng et al., 2004). However, another CUB domain protein, Neto2

regulates mammalian kainate receptor trafficking and gating ( Zhang et al., 2009). In addition, buy Screening Library studies have found recently that another AMPA receptor auxiliary subunit, CKAMP44, associates with AMPA receptors and reduces currents ( von Engelhardt et al., 2010). Multiple auxiliary subunits regulate trafficking and gating of voltage-gated calcium channels, and the α2δ subunit also controls the pharmacology of certain calcium channel compounds ( Gee et al., 1996). As AMPA receptor modulators show therapeutic potential in numerous neuropsychiatric disorders ( Kato and Bredt,

2007), TARP and CNIH proteins provide intriguing pharmacological targets. All salts, precast gels, and buffers were from Sigma Capmatinib datasheet Aldrich (St. Louis, MO), Invitrogen (Carlsbad, CA), Fisher Scientific (Pittsburgh, PA), or Bio-Rad Laboratories (Hercules, CA). Antagonist and agonists were from Tocris Bioscience (Ellisville, MO). Polyclonal antibodies against GluK2/3 (04-921), pan-Type I TARP (07-577), and GluA1 (AB1504) and monoclonal antibody against GluR2 (MAB3397) were purchased from Millipore (Billerica, MA). Mouse monoclonal PSD-95 antibody (MA1-046) and polyclonal antibody against PICK-1 (PAI-073) were purchased from Affinity Bioreagents (Rockford, IL). Mouse monoclonal synaptophysin antibody (S5768) was purchased from Sigma-Aldrich (St. Louis, MO). Mouse monoclonal antibody against NR1 (556308) was purchased from BD PharMingen (San Jose, CA). Metalloexopeptidase Affinity-purified

polyclonal antibodies for CNIH-2 were generated by immunizing guinea pigs with the following peptide sequence from human CNIH-2 protein, DELRTDFKNPIDQGNPARARERLKNIERIC. HRP-conjugated anti-guinea pig secondary antibody (706-035-148) and HRP-conjugated native secondary antibody for mouse- and rabbit-derived primary antibodies (21230) were from Jackson Laboratories (West Grove, PA) and Fisher Scientific, respectively. All GluA cDNAs are flip splice variants unless indicated. All GluA and TARP cDNAs were derived from human except for GluA2, which was cloned from rat. shRNA producing plasmids and lentiviral particles were purchased from Sigma-Aldrich. (#1: TRCN0000109842, #2: TRCN0000109844). HEK293T cells were maintained at 37°C in 5% CO2 high glucose DMEM medium supplemented with 10% fetal calf serum and 1% penicillin-streptomycin and split bi- or triweekly.

These observations suggest a distinction between goal directednes

These observations suggest a distinction between goal directedness and deliberation scales for understanding find more an action sequence as a habit. At a mechanistic level, we found aspects of DLS activity that accord with it storing cached values, in that the task-bracketing activity formed early and was maintained across changes in outcome value as though ready to influence behavior whenever selected. However, surprisingly, DLS activity was most clearly related

to the amount of deliberation rather than to other variables. Its task-bracketing activity not only remained fixed when values and behavior first changed after devaluation but even after new values had been incorporated into a putative second habit. The dominant task-bracketing ensemble spike activity pattern in the DLS might therefore not relate to specific S-R associations, which would probably have changed as the second habit overtook the first one. Some units might still retain such S-R associations but might be in the minority, in accord with observations in related work

(Berke et al., 2009, de Wit et al., 2011, Root et al., 2010 and Thorn et al., Screening Library cell line 2010). Our findings, instead, link the DLS bracketing pattern to the automatic execution of a familiar course of action, almost irrespective of actual outcome value or route-related details once the pattern is acquired. One interesting possibility is that this pattern represents a value bound to the learned behavior that has been bracketed, as though through the reinforcement history the behavior itself had grown to be an incentive (Glickman and Schiff, 1967). Other open alternatives include that however the pattern reflected a stored S-R value of initially learned runs only, that S-R representations occurred in features of activity not assessed here, or that sensory stimuli in the maze environment guided behavior apart from instrumental processes despite the shift from outcome-sensitive to outcome-insensitive performance. For the IL cortex, the close relationship

between task-bracketing activity and the expression of outcome-insensitive behavior is consistent with its participation in an executive control process that selects habits. We found, however, that this relationship did not hold uniformly at the level of individual instances of execution of the behavior. If the IL cortex were an arbiter, it might be expected to “choose” the habitual or nonhabitual mode on any given trial (Wunderlich et al., 2012), but its activity did not suggest this. IL activity instead appeared to result in a general state permissive of habitual behaviors; it tracked, in general, the goal directedness of the behavior but not the detailed S-R type of behavior usually considered as a habit.